🐒Animal Behavior Unit 6 – Optimal Foraging Strategies
Optimal foraging theory predicts how animals maximize energy intake while minimizing costs. Animals make decisions based on factors like prey abundance, handling time, and search time. These strategies, shaped by natural selection, balance energy intake with risks like predation and competition.
Key concepts include the marginal value theorem, which predicts when animals leave food patches, and the diet breadth model, which explains specialization versus generalization. Foraging strategies vary widely, from sit-and-wait predators to active foragers, and are influenced by factors like habitat structure and climate.
Generalists have a broad diet and can switch between food sources depending on availability (raccoons, bears)
Specialists have a narrow diet and rely on specific food sources (koalas, pandas)
Central place foragers return to a fixed location (nest, den) between foraging bouts (bees, ants)
Cooperative foragers work together to locate and capture prey (lions, killer whales)
Cost-Benefit Analysis
Animals must weigh the benefits of obtaining food against the costs associated with foraging
Energy intake is the primary benefit, as it provides the necessary resources for growth, maintenance, and reproduction
Costs include energy expenditure, time spent foraging, and the risk of injury or predation
Optimal foraging decisions maximize the net energy gain (benefits minus costs) per unit time
Handling time is the time required to capture, subdue, and consume a prey item
Example: A bird spending more time cracking open a tough seed (handling time) may be less efficient than one consuming many small, easily accessible seeds
Search time is the time spent looking for prey between successful captures
Example: A predator may spend more time searching for rare, high-quality prey (large mammals) compared to abundant, low-quality prey (insects)
Risk of predation can influence foraging decisions, as animals may avoid areas or times of day with high predator activity
Opportunity costs arise when choosing one foraging option precludes the ability to exploit other options simultaneously
Mathematical Models
Optimal foraging theory uses mathematical models to predict how animals should behave to maximize their fitness
Marginal value theorem (MVT) predicts when an animal should leave a patch based on the diminishing returns of energy intake over time
The optimal leaving time occurs when the instantaneous rate of energy gain equals the average rate for the environment
MVT equation: dtdE=ti+t0Ei−E0, where Ei is the energy intake in patch i, E0 is the energy intake between patches, ti is the time spent in patch i, and t0 is the travel time between patches
Giving up density (GUD) is the amount of food remaining in a patch when an animal decides to leave
GUD provides a measure of the perceived costs (predation risk, missed opportunities) associated with foraging in a patch
Lower GUD indicates higher perceived costs, as the animal is willing to leave more food behind
Diet breadth model predicts the optimal number of prey types an animal should include in its diet based on their profitability (energy gain per unit handling time)
Animals should specialize on high-profitability prey when abundant and generalize to include lower-profitability prey when preferred prey become scarce
Diet breadth equation: D=hiei, where D is the diet breadth, ei is the energy gained from prey type i, and hi is the handling time for prey type i
Case Studies
Bluegill sunfish (Lepomis macrochirus) optimize their foraging by selecting prey based on size and density
In experiments, bluegills consistently chose the most profitable prey (highest energy gain per unit handling time) when presented with a choice
As the density of preferred prey decreased, bluegills expanded their diet to include less profitable prey, as predicted by the diet breadth model
Bumblebees (Bombus spp.) use a combination of learning and memory to optimize their foraging on flower patches
Bumblebees learn the locations of profitable flower patches and adjust their foraging routes to minimize travel time between patches
They also learn the refilling rates of nectar in flowers and time their visits accordingly to maximize energy intake
Starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) optimize their foraging by adjusting their patch residence time based on prey density and handling time
In experiments, starlings stayed longer in patches with higher prey density and shorter handling times, as predicted by the marginal value theorem
Starlings also demonstrated the ability to assess patch quality and modify their foraging behavior in response to changes in prey availability
Coyotes (Canis latrans) adapt their foraging strategies based on the availability and distribution of prey
In areas with abundant small mammals (rodents), coyotes act as specialized predators, focusing their efforts on these high-profitability prey
In areas where small mammals are scarce, coyotes become generalist predators, including a wider variety of prey in their diet (rabbits, insects, fruits)
Environmental Factors
Prey availability and distribution influence foraging strategies, as animals must adjust their behavior to match the abundance and accessibility of food resources
Example: In seasons or years with high prey abundance, predators may specialize on preferred prey, while in times of scarcity, they may switch to alternative prey or expand their diet breadth
Habitat structure and complexity can affect foraging efficiency, as animals must navigate and search for prey in different environments
Example: Aquatic predators (fish) may use different foraging strategies in open water (active pursuit) compared to structured habitats like coral reefs (ambush tactics)
Climate and weather conditions can impact foraging success, as they affect prey activity, visibility, and the energetic costs of foraging
Example: In cold temperatures, endothermic animals (mammals, birds) may need to increase their foraging efforts to maintain their body temperature, while ectothermic animals (reptiles, amphibians) may become less active
Competition among individuals or species can lead to niche partitioning and specialized foraging strategies to reduce overlap and maximize resource utilization
Example: Different species of warblers (Parulidae) forage at different heights and locations within a tree to minimize competition for insects
Anthropogenic factors, such as habitat modification, pollution, and climate change, can disrupt optimal foraging strategies and force animals to adapt to novel conditions
Example: Urban birds (pigeons, sparrows) may rely on human food scraps and waste as a significant portion of their diet, leading to changes in their foraging behavior and population dynamics
Applications and Implications
Understanding optimal foraging strategies can inform conservation and management decisions for threatened or endangered species
Example: Identifying key foraging habitats and prey species can help prioritize areas for protection and restoration
Foraging theory can be applied to the design of agricultural systems and pest control strategies
Example: Planting trap crops or diversifying farm landscapes can manipulate the foraging behavior of insect pests and reduce damage to main crops
Insights from optimal foraging can be used to develop more efficient algorithms for search and resource allocation problems in computer science and engineering
Example: Ant colony optimization (ACO) algorithms mimic the foraging behavior of ants to find optimal solutions to complex problems like routing and scheduling
Studying foraging behavior can provide a window into the cognitive abilities and decision-making processes of animals
Example: Experiments testing the ability of animals to assess patch quality, remember food locations, and adjust their strategies based on experience can shed light on their learning and memory capabilities
Foraging theory can be extended to other domains, such as human behavior and economics, to understand how individuals make decisions when faced with resource constraints and trade-offs
Example: Consumer choice theory in economics draws on principles of optimal foraging to explain how people allocate their time and money among different goods and services based on their perceived value and costs
Climate change and habitat loss may alter the availability and distribution of prey, requiring animals to adapt their foraging strategies or face population declines
Example: Polar bears (Ursus maritimus) are shifting their foraging behavior in response to declining sea ice, spending more time on land and targeting alternative prey like birds and eggs
Invasive species can disrupt native foraging patterns and compete for resources, leading to changes in community structure and ecosystem function
Example: The introduction of the brown tree snake (Boiga irregularis) to Guam has decimated native bird populations, altering the foraging behavior of remaining species and the dispersal of seeds and insects